Threlfell et al (2012) also demonstrated that optical stimulatio

Threlfell et al. (2012) also demonstrated that optical stimulation of thalamic projections to ChIs could mimic the effect of direct activation of ChIs, raising the remarkable possibility that the thalamic intralaminar nuclei can control DA release—a situation they are well positioned to do, given their sensitivity to salient stimuli (Matsumoto selleck chemicals llc et al., 2001). The paper in Cell Reports by Cheer’s group ( Cachope et al., 2012) describes a very similar

scenario in the ventral striatum-nucleus accumbens (NAc). In addition to showing that optogenetic stimulation of a population of ChIs induces DA release in slices of the NAc, Cachope et al. show that the same thing happens in vivo. One apparent point of divergence with the Threlfell et al. work is the

inferred role of glutamate. Threlfell et al. found no effect of glutamate receptor antagonists on the ChI-induced DA release, but Cheer’s group did find a partial reduction in release with the antagonism of AMPA receptors. They linked this to the recently described corelease of glutamate and ACh by ChIs ( Higley et al., 2011). However, because this release is rapidly LY294002 cell line lost at the normal spiking rates of ChIs, its enhancement of DA release would be limited to rebound spiking after a long pause. How does Threlfell et al.’s discovery change our understanding of the striatum? First, their work has vindicated the findings of the French group led by Glowinski, who emphasized intrastriatal

control of DA release decades ago but who had few adherents. Clearly, DA release is not driven solely by the substantia nigra. Quite remarkably, even the thalamus can drive DA release in the striatum through the mechanism that Cragg and colleagues have outlined. almost Moreover, the model that DA and ACh simply oppose one another—as in the feud metaphor—needs to be fundamentally revised. A revision doesn’t mean, however, that these two neurotransmitters are bosom buddies. DA does suppress ACh release, even if the converse is not true. Moreover, there is still compelling evidence that DA and ACh can have opposed effects on striatal physiology. For example, the induction of long-term depression at corticostriatal synapses of principal spiny projection neurons (SPNs) is promoted by an elevation in DA and a fall in ACh (Bagetta et al., 2011 and Wang et al., 2006). In indirect pathway SPNs that express D2 DA receptors, DA clearly depresses intrinsic excitability, and ACh increases it through activation of M1 muscarinic receptors (Gerfen and Surmeier, 2010). In direct pathway SPNs that express D1 DA receptors, the situation appears to be more nuanced by the coexpression of M1 and M4 muscarinic receptors. In vitro studies of ACh regulation of DA have been plagued by the difficulty in selectively stimulating particular microcircuits. When and where you stimulate matters, as shown by both Threlfell et al. and Cachope et al.

Under these conditions, AAs activated a net inward current, which

Under these conditions, AAs activated a net inward current, which did not reverse within the membrane potential range we examined (Figure 4B). Because such current-voltage characteristics resemble those of electrogenic amino acid transporters, whose activity depolarizes cell membranes due to cotransport of Na+ ions (Mackenzie and Erickson, 2004 and Mackenzie et al., 2003), we tested the effects of different blockers

of these membrane transporters. The excitatory amino acid transporter blocker TBOA did not affect the tolbutamide-insensitive selleck kinase inhibitor remnant of the AA response (Figure 4D). In contrast, the system-A transporter inhibitor meAIB completely abolished it (Figures 4C and 4D). Together, these data imply that membrane depolarization induced

by AAs is explained by a decrease in hyperpolarizing activity of tolbutamide-sensitive KATP channels and a concurrent increase in the depolarizing activity of meAIB-sensitive system-A transporters. We next examined the intracellular signaling pathways involved in AA sensing. We focused on ATP-generating pathways potentially coupled to KATP channels, and on mTOR-requiring pathways, which may mediate AA sensing in other hypothalamic regions (Cota et al., 2006). Suppressing mitochondrial ATP production Talazoparib with 2 μM oligomycin reduced, but did not abolish, the effect of AAs on the membrane potential and current (Figures 5A and 5C). The current-voltage relationship of the oligomycin-insensitive component of the AA response (Figure 5A) was similar to the tolbutamide-insensitive

component (Figure 4B), suggesting that the two drugs block the same part of the response. The simplest explanation for this is that mitochondria-derived ATP is required to drive the KATP -dependent component of the AA response. In contrast, blocking mTOR activity Calpain with 1 μM rapamycin did not affect AA-induced depolarization or current (n = 5, Figure 5B,C), suggesting that mTOR is not critical for AA sensing in orx/hcrt neurons, consistent with the lack of effect of leucine (an mTOR stimulator) on orx/hcrt cells (Figures 3C, 3E, and 3F). There is evidence suggesting that brain levels of both glucose and AAs may rise after a meal (Choi et al., 1999, Choi et al., 2000 and Silver and Erecińska, 1994). We therefore examined the effects of simultaneous application of glucose and AAs. We expected that when AAs and glucose are applied together, the two responses would either cancel out or produce a net inhibition because the inhibitory current induced by glucose (e.g., see Figure 7A) was generally larger than the excitatory current induced by AAs (e.g., see Figure 4A).

That firing rates do not adapt to zero but rather to a relatively

That firing rates do not adapt to zero but rather to a relatively high rate indicates that trafficking (superlinear component) is rapidly accessible under physiological conditions. Similar to the response described in Figure 5, under physiological conditions the processes tend to merge but vesicle release shows a reduction in slope initially that becomes sustained. The level of neural adaptation may in part be determined by how rapidly each synapse is capable of recruiting

vesicles between pools—the faster the recruitment, selleckchem the less adaptation is observed. In fact, it may be argued that steady-state firing requires recruitment of vesicles such that the rate of release at any given synapse may be dictated by access to the reserve pool of vesicles. Thus it may be that spontaneous firing rates are regulated

by resting calcium currents and vesicles in the RRP and recycling pool, while stimulated release is more dependent upon vesicle recruitment from the reserve pool and the ability to modulate release of stored calcium (Guth et al., 1991). In summary, we used real-time capacitance measurements to identify saturable pools of vesicles and discovered a superlinear release component requiring recruitment of vesicles to release sites. We suggest that Ca2+-dependent vesicle trafficking MAPK inhibitor is responsible for this movement, which is required for hair cell synapses to maintain high rates of sustained vesicle fusion. We postulate that the superlinear release component reflects synapses operating at maximal rates of release and trafficking Dichloromethane dehalogenase and that release of an as yet undefined internal pool of Ca2+ may be required. These characteristics of synaptic vesicle recruitment and release make hair cell ribbon

synapses quite unique as compared to other synapses. The auditory papilla from red-eared sliders (Trachemys scripta elegans) were prepared as previously described ( Schnee et al., 2005) by using methods approved by the IACUC committee at Stanford University and following standards established by NIH guidelines. Tectorial membranes were removed as previously described by using a hypertonic and hypercalcemic (10 mM Ca2+) solution ( Farris et al., 2006). The external recording solution contained 125 mM NaCl, 0.5 mM KCl, 2.8 mM CaCl2, 2.2 mM MgCl2, 2 mM pyruvate, 2 mM creatine, 2 mM ascorbate, 6 mM glucose, and 10 mM N-(2-hydroxy-ethyl) piperazine-N′-(2-ethanesulfonic acid) (HEPES) with pH adjusted to 7.6 and osmolality maintained at 275 mosml/kg. One hundred nanometers apamin was included in the external solution to block SK potassium currents ( Tucker and Fettiplace, 1996). Cells were imaged with a BX51 fixed-stage upright microscope (Olympus) with bright-field optics. Conventional epifluorescence was used for the Ca2+ imaging.

bassiana oil-based formulations ( Table 1) The formulations of M

bassiana oil-based formulations ( Table 1). The formulations of M. anisopliae s.l. containing 10, 15, or 20% mineral oil showed control percentages of 58.1, 93.7 and 87.5%, respectively while the aqueous suspension produced 18.7% control ( Table 2). From all the groups treated with B. bassiana, the 10 and 20% oil formulations showed the highest efficacy achieving 18.1 and 21.6% control, respectively ( Table 2). The M. anisopliae oil-based formulations significantly reduced (p < 0.01; df = 7) the incubation period, hatching period, and hatching percentage when compared to the other treatments ( Table 3). The hatching percentage in the GDC-0973 in vivo groups treated with the M. anisopliae s.l. oil-based formulations was reduced as much

as 102.5 times. In contrast, the M. anisopliae

s.l. aqueous suspension significantly reduced (p < 0.05; df = 7) the hatching period and hatching percentage as compared to the aqueous control group ( Table 3). Neither the B. bassiana aqueous suspension nor the oil-based formulations significantly affected (p < 0.05; df = 7) the incubation period when R. microplus eggs were exposed to that fungus ( Table 3). A significant reduction (p < 0.05; df = 7) in the hatching period appeared to be related with exposure to the B. bassiana aqueous suspension and B. bassiana oil-based formulations when compared with the results for the aqueous control group; however, the effect was not observed when results from the same treatments were compared to data obtained for the oil-based control groups ( Table 3). The B. bassiana aqueous suspension caused no OSI 906 change from in the percentage of larvae hatching. By

contrast, the oil-based formulations significantly reduced (p < 0.05; df = 7) this parameter (27–47.5%) when compared to the control groups (93.5–98.4%) ( Table 3). The oil-based formulations of M. anisopliae s.l. and B. bassiana were more efficient in controlling R. microplus larvae as compared to the aqueous suspensions ( Fig. 1). The mean mortality rate for larvae treated with M. anisopliae s.l. oil-based formulations was close to nearly 100% on the fifth day after treatment while the aqueous fungal suspension caused 2.0% larval mortality ( Fig. 1A). B. bassiana treatments started to cause noticeable larval mortality the tenth day after treatment ( Fig. 1D). Mean larval mortality with the B. bassiana oil-based formulations was close to 100% at 20 days after treatment while the aqueous suspension caused only 27.4% larval mortality ( Fig. 1F). The control group receiving the control treatment containing 20% mineral oil showed average mortality rates of 28.1, 40.9, and 41.3% on the 15, 20, and 25th days after treatment, respectively (Fig. 1E, F and G). A significant larval mortality rate was observed in the control formulation with 10 or 15% oil on the 20 and 25th days after treatment (Fig. 1F and G). No larval mortality was observed in the control group treated with water.

Given that cells with high firing rate are often interneurons, we

Given that cells with high firing rate are often interneurons, we divided the

check details PL neurons into putative pyramidal cells and interneurons by carrying out an unsupervised cluster analysis of the cells based on their firing rates and spike widths (Letzkus et al., 2011; see Experimental Procedures for details). This procedure yielded two main clusters (Figure 1D). Consistent with previous reports, one cluster contained a majority of neurons with low firing rate (<15 Hz) and broad spike waveform (>225 μs; putative excitatory pyramidal neurons), while the other contained neurons with high firing rates (>15 Hz) and narrow spike waveforms (<225 μs; putative inhibitory interneurons). Of 194 PL neurons, 174 (89.7%) were classified as putative pyramidal neurons, and 20 (10.3%) were classified as putative interneurons. Similar proportions of interneurons in the rat prefrontal cortex have been previously reported (Homayoun and Moghaddam, 2007). In support of our classification, we observed significant inhibitory interactions between putative inhibitory neurons and putative pyramidal cells, as evidenced by cross-correlation analyses MEK pathway (n = 10 of 88 pairs of neurons; Figure 1E). Classification of PL neurons into pyramidal cells and interneurons revealed dissociable effects of BLA and vHPC inputs. We limited our analysis to PL cells that exhibited significant changes in firing rate following input inactivation (p < 0.05, n = 108/194).

This additional restriction eliminated the few putative pyramidal cells that fell below the firing rate/waveform length criterion. Inactivation of BLA significantly decreased the firing rate of pyramidal neurons (n = 52; Wilcoxon test: Z = 2.57, p = 0.010), without affecting interneuron activity (n = 6; Wilcoxon test: Z = 1.36, p = 0.17; Figure 1F), suggesting that BLA input to PL is largely excitatory in conditioned

rats pressing for food. In contrast, inactivation of vHPC significantly decreased the firing rate of interneurons tuclazepam (n = 7; Wilcoxon test: Z = 2.36, p = 0.01), without affecting pyramidal cell activity (n = 43; Wilcoxon test: Z = 1.48, p = 0.13; Figure 1G). This suggests that vHPC inputs are capable of triggering feed-forward inhibition of PL neurons by exciting local interneurons. This difference between BLA and vHPC inputs could not be predicted from prior anatomical ( Carr and Sesack, 1996; Gabbott et al., 2002, 2006; Hoover and Vertes, 2007; McDonald, 1991) or physiological ( Dégenètais et al., 2003; Floresco and Tse, 2007; Laviolette et al., 2005; Tierney et al., 2004) studies. We next assessed the effect of input inactivation on tone responses of PL neurons, tested after conditioning. Conditioned tone tests occurred from 2 hr to several days postconditioning (see Experimental Procedures for details). Unilateral inactivation of either BLA or vHPC did not alter tone evoked freezing in conditioned rats (see Figure S1 available online).

An obvious proautophagic candidate drug would be rapamycin, which

An obvious proautophagic candidate drug would be rapamycin, which has already been shown to protect against neuronal death in mouse models of PD (Malagelada et al., 2010). For mutations in other genes associated with mitochondrial function, and especially those that impair function only partially, a third promising

approach might be to increase energy production in patients by upregulating PGC-1α expression using compounds such as bezafibrate, a PPAR panagonist (Santra et al., 2004), or 5-aminoimidazole-4-carboxamide ribonucleoside (AICAR), which acts as an AMP agonist by mimicking AMP (Viscomi et al., 2011). Finally, it may be possible to alter mitodynamics directly by, for example, shifting the relationship between fission and fusion pharmacologically, MLN0128 supplier using the quinazolinone mitochondrial division inhibitor 1 (mdivi1), which enhances mitochondrial

click here fusion in yeast by inhibiting the mitochondrial dynamin Dnm1 that is required for organelle fission (Cassidy-Stone et al., 2008). We may view the role of mitochondria in the pathogenesis of neurodegenerative disorders, and the ways in which we have begun to think about therpaeutics, as multifaceted, and going well beyond the “mere” synthesis and distribution of ATP throughout cells. Mitochondria encompass numerous functions, including many important ones that have not even been discussed here (e.g., amino acid metabolism, steroid metabolism, apoptosis, xenobiotic detoxification, and immunological defense), all of which could play a role in neurodegenerative disorders. To the cliché

that mitochondria are the powerhouses of the cell, let us add one more: what has been uncovered in Phosphoprotein phosphatase the last 10 years regarding the role of mitochondria in neurodegenerative disorders is merely the tip of the iceberg. Far more exciting findings lay ahead. We thank Drs. William Dauer, Salvatore DiMauro, Michio Hirano, Peter Hollenbeck, Orian Shirihai, and Jean Paul Vonsattel for critical comments, and Robert Lee and Arnaud Jacquier for their expert assistance with the figure. This work was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (HD32062 to E.A.S.; and NS042269, NS064191, NS38370, NS070276, and NS072182 to S.P.), the U.S. Department of Defense (W81XWH-08-1-0522, W81XWH-08-1-0465, and W81XWH-09-1-0245 to S.P.), the Parkinson Disease Foundation, the Thomas Hartman Foundation For Parkinson’s Research, Project A.L.S, the Muscular Dystrophy Association, the Ellison Medical Foundation, the Alzheimer Drug Discovery Foundation, and the Marriott Mitochondrial Disorder Clinical Research Fund (MMDCRF). “
“Recent years have witnessed a great surge of interest in understanding the neural mechanisms of reward-guided learning and decision-making.

A possible explanation is that motion-evoked release of ACh from

A possible explanation is that motion-evoked release of ACh from SACs onto DSGCs is functionally asymmetric, but the cholinergic synaptic connectivity is anatomically symmetric (see below). The finding of directional asymmetry in the NMDA component but not the AMPA/KA component raised the possibility that the fast, direction-selective nicotinic input might act synergistically with a

direction-selective NMDA input to provide an associative excitation that helps the cell overcome the voltage-dependent Mg2+ blockade of NMDA receptors during the preferred direction movement (Figure S1, available online). However, the difference in direction selectivity between NMDA and AMPA/KA components remains to be understood. In addition to the opposite directional NVP-BKM120 mw asymmetry, the light-evoked GABAergic input and the HEX-sensitive input to a DSGC also differ dramatically in spatial extent. The GABAergic input could be evoked from the null side when the leading edge of a moving

bar was as far as 150 μm (ranging from 30 to 150 μm, with a mean ± standard deviation [SD] of 64 ± 39 μm, n = 53) from the edge of the DSGC’s dendritic field (Figure 3A), consistent with it being a leading lateral inhibition from SACs (Fried et al., Ku-0059436 solubility dmso 2002). In contrast, the excitatory inputs, including the HEX-sensitive input, were restricted within the

dendritic field of the DSGC (n = 12, Figure 3A), as previously reported (Fried et al., 2002, Fried et al., 2005, Taylor and Vaney, 2002, Yang and Masland, 1992 and Yang and Masland, 1994). To understand the spatial properties of the cholinergic receptive field (RF) of a DSGC, a two-spot apparent motion paradigm was used. Flashing a stationary light spot in the RF surround could not evoke a detectable HEX-sensitive EPSC (Figure 4A), suggesting that the nicotinic inputs unless formed a silent excitatory surround, which did not produce a leading lateral excitation during stimulus movement. This result is consistent with a previous report that the Off cholinergic input to DGGC also does not show an extended surround (Fried et al., 2005). However, when two stationary spots were flashed in a quick succession to simulate a preferred-direction movement, the first flash (in RF surround, which by itself did not evoke a cholinergic response) greatly facilitated the HEX-sensitive response to the second flash (in RF center, Figures 4A and 4B), indicating that ACh release was facilitated by stimulus motion. This new finding provided a synaptic basis for the suggestion that ACh facilitates motion detection (Chiao and Masland, 2002 and He and Masland, 1997).

Similarly, in the phenomenon of induction, in which a temporally

Similarly, in the phenomenon of induction, in which a temporally varying surround region induces an illusory Dolutegravir cost modulation of a constant center region, the perceived modulation depth of the center is significantly attenuated at high surround TFs. However, when two high TFs are summed and presented in the surround, the center is perceived to modulate at the envelope frequency (D’Antona and Shevell, 2009). The

present results thus suggest that a subcortical demodulating nonlinearity allows high TF information that is otherwise lost in the geniculocortical transformation to affect cortical firing patterns, and possibly perception. Non-Fourier signals are generally associated with the detection of oriented contours and the processing of texture (Rivest and Cavanagh, 1996 and Song and Baker, 2007), but they also arise at occlusion boundaries and under conditions producing transparent motion (Fleet and Langley, 1994). Both occlusion boundaries and transparent motion, the DAPT perception of multiple velocity signals in a local area of retinotopic space (Qian and Andersen, 1994), provide monocular cues for depth order. Non-Fourier signals can consequently elicit salient depth perceptions from non-stereoscopic

stimuli (Hegdé et al., 2004); for instance, the envelope of an interference pattern can be perceived to drift in front of the carrier (Fleet and Langley, 1994; Figure S6). The tuning of Y cells for both the envelope TF and the carrier TF of interference patterns (Figures S5A and S5B) therefore constitutes a joint representation of motions occupying an overlapping area of retinotopic space that can be perceived to be at different depths. Although the processing of occlusion boundaries and transparent why motion is commonly associated with extrastriate cortex (Qian and Andersen, 1994 and Rosenberg et al., 2008), the results of the present study suggest that some aspects of these signals are first represented subcortically.

All procedures were approved by the University of Chicago Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. These methods have been described previously (Rosenberg et al., 2010 and Zhang et al., 2007) and are summarized here. All experiments were performed in anesthetized adult female cats. Baytril (2.5–5 mg/kg SQ) was given as prophylaxis against infection, dexamethasone (1–2 mg/kg SQ) was given to reduce cerebral edema, and atropine (0.04 mg/kg SQ) was given to decrease tracheal secretions. Ophthalmic atropine (1%) and phenylephrine (10%) were instilled in the eyes to dilate the pupils and retract the nictitating membrane, respectively. Lactated Ringer’s Solution (LRS) with 2.5% dextrose was delivered IV at a rate of 2–10 ml/kg/hr. Pancuronium bromide (0.1 mg/kg loading dose, 0.04–0.125 mg/kg/hr continuous) was given IV as a paralytic and delivered in the LRS.

We found that PPC cells were tuned to totally different behaviors

We found that PPC cells were tuned to totally different behaviors in the hairpin maze and open field, and recordings in the virtual hairpin showed that restructuring the animals’ behavior was the primary factor in driving the cells to retune. While we acknowledge that changes in locomotor behavior alone likely account for only a fraction of the variability observed in the PPC cell population, the data suggest

HCS assay nevertheless that engaging an animal in a goal-driven task alters the way PPC cells represent an animal’s state of motion. As there was no change in local sensory inputs between the open field and virtual hairpin, it is possible that the retuning of the cells was driven by inputs from neural populations mediating the cognitive demands of the task. The similarity of the PPC representations between the virtual hairpin and hairpin maze suggests that the cells’ responses were shaped

by the similar behavioral constraints of the two tasks, and may imply that comparable anatomical inputs were at play in driving the cells in each condition. The retuning of PPC cells between the open field and virtual hairpin demonstrates that the way in which the cells represented locomotor actions changed depending on the task in Carfilzomib manufacturer which the actions were embedded. This finding is conceptually similar to observations in mirror neurons in primates, where cells in the inferior parietal lobule distinguished between similar grasping movements depending on the intended goal of the movement (Fogassi et al., 2005). In terms of navigation, prior studies established that PPC cells encode sequences of movements in a route-specific manner (Sato et al., 2006 and Nitz, 2006). Our results for add to these findings by showing that PPC cells encode movements differently depending on the structure of the animals’ behavior per se, in the absence of any physical maze, and support the interpretation that the parietal contribution to navigation has

more to do with the organization of actions than the formation of a spatial image. A central aim of this study was to discern whether representations in PPC and MEC were expressed synchronously or in parallel. PPC cells expressed firing fields corresponding to translational movements irrespective of an animal’s location, whereas grid cells in MEC expressed spatial maps independently of the animals’ state of motion. Representations in both PPC and MEC were affected when the animals were placed in the hairpin maze, with cells in PPC switching behavioral correlates completely and grid cells showing a fragmentation of the hexagonal structure of their firing fields. We tested the effect of manipulating spatial inputs outside the task by running the animals in hairpin mazes in two different rooms and found that PPC cells retained their firing preferences despite a complete reorganization of grid cell firing fields.

In addition to electrical coupling with AVA, A motoneurons also r

In addition to electrical coupling with AVA, A motoneurons also receive excitatory chemical synaptic inputs from AVA and AVE (Figure 1B). Hyperactivated backward premotor interneurons in innexin mutants could therefore lead to an increased chemical synaptic output to A motoneurons and contribute to their preference for backing. Indeed, when we silenced the activity of premotor interneurons of the backward circuit and PVC by Pnmr-1::TWK-18(gf) Imatinib solubility dmso ( Figure S4), hyperactivated backing in these innexin mutants was effectively prevented ( Figure S5B; Movie S5, parts B–D). Such an effect was mimicked by expressing tetanus

toxin, a specific blocker of chemical synapses ( Macosko et al., 2009) in the same Selleckchem Dinaciclib set of premotor interneurons ( Figure S5B; Movie S5, part E). Both Pnmr-1::TWK-18(gf) ( Figure S5B; Movie S5, part A) and Pnmr-1-Tetanus toxin ( Movie S5, part F) prevented

continuous backing in wild-type animals. These results further support the idea that chemical synaptic output from backward premotor interneurons is required to sustain backing. Together these results indicate that AVA-A coupling acts as shunts to dampen the activity of backward premotor interneurons in wild-type animals, which reduces their chemical synaptic inputs onto A motoneurons and prevents the hyperactivation of backing. Reducing backward premotor interneuron activity constitutes only half of the role of AVA-A coupling in promoting forward movement. Although the AVA/AVE-silencing transgene effectively inhibited backing in innexin mutants (Figure S5B), it did not suppress kinking: these animals still adopted a kinked posture (Figure S5A, bottom middle) instead of moving forward (Figure S5B; Movie S5, parts B–D). Consistently, although they no longer generated Ribonucleotide reductase the backing-associated A > B pattern, they continued to establish the A = B pattern (Figures 8A–8A″).

This contrasted the case in wild-type background, in which inactivating AVA/AVE by the same transgene led to an exclusive B > A pattern (Figures 8A–8A″) and forward movement (Figures S5A and S5B; Movie S5, part A). The failure to further reduce A activity when AVA were silenced (Figures 8A–8A″; Figure S4) is consistent with the notion that AVA and A are uncoupled in these innexin mutants. However, observing a persistent A motoneuron activity in the presence of this transgene was unexpected because silencing AVA and AVE eliminates both chemical and electrical synaptic inputs to A motoneurons (Figure 1B). The residual A motoneuron activity may therefore represent a premotor interneuron-independent (referred to as endogenous) motoneuron activity that is suppressed by their coupling with AVA to allow the establishment of a B > A output pattern in wild-type animals.